First Academic Journal Paper Published

EAR article image
Our first academic journal article has been published in Educational Action Research, August 2017

Our first academic journal paper from the Creative Science at Life research and development project has been published in Educational Action Research, in August 2017. Published by Routledge, the journal is concerned with exploring the dialogue between research and practice in educational settings. The focus of the paper is on the use of participatory action research (PAR) to enable university researchers and Science Centre professionals to co-design Informal Science Learning exhibits that enhance creativity and innovation in young people. We discuss how PAR enabled effective engagement with and creation of enriched knowledge and innovation, in both the academy and science-learning professionals. The added value of PAR and co-production to our project aligns with current calls in academia for a redefining of how societal impact of academic research is considered.

The paper adds to the impact of the project, and can be read in full and downloaded.

Advertisements

Practical science and the messy data of children’s building blocks

Scientific theories can often be elegant, but the underbelly of scientific research is frequently messy, tiring work, and thoroughly inelegant. This is data collection. The same is true of cooking. No matter how beautifully planned a recipe can be on paper, inevitable practicalities always require pragmatic choices in its preparation. In carrying out our own data collection for the collaborative condition of our physical experiment at Newcastle’s Centre for Life, we encountered the challenging and thought-provoking practicalities that arise when working with children in the sometimes chaotic, constantly moving environment of the gallery floor. These are our experiences.

The data we collected are messy, in other words they are variable. Our idea was to get two, three, or more children to stand around a table and work together (the collaborative condition of our physical experiment) to construct something in the centre, using wooden blocks. This rarely occurred. Instead, with the provision of a footstool for smaller participants, the children frequently clustered around the protruding ‘peninsulas’ of our table. Or alternatively they took no permanent position and instead darted to and fro around the table’s edges to occupy different vantage points on their constructions, almost as bees construct their hive.

Messy data means there are multiple variables at play in each building group we observed. Children’s ages and sexes varied, as did their relations to each other (siblings, friends, strangers etc.). It is the job of data analysis to separate these variables out by comparing one group to another. We need this analysis because of a trade off we made between control over the experiment and the relevance of the experiment to the ‘real’ environment. By using children’s participation in a science centre exhibit as our experiment, we abstracted only somewhat from children’s ‘natural’ interactions with museum exhibits. We can therefore claim high relevance to the environment (‘ecological validity’), but we accordingly need intensive analysis after data collection to distinguish the variables at play.

In fact the varied behaviours of the children we observed appeared so interesting and accessible for data collection that we drew up a simple list of familiar behaviours (an ‘ethogram’) which we could note down as and when we saw them performed by the children. This neatly illustrates our pragmatic approach to data collection since the original idea was to focus just on the things children created from blocks. Among other variables, we collected information about where around the table children were standing, whenever they copied from each other, and each time they discussed their building or gave instructions.

By expanding into recording behaviours we hope to make use of these extra data to illuminate different perspectives on the findings from our first interest in the buildings themselves. Looking at behaviours allows us to ask questions such as: What is the impact of discussion on children’s innovation in building? Does the amount of copying correlate to different building types? and, What are the effects when children provide instruction to others? Asking these questions allows us a more comprehensive view of children’s collaborative building with blocks.

At the end of each build, we offered the children a sticker as a prize for participating. We give the child the sticker as a gift in return for our use of the data they have provided. It illustrates for the child that they have done us a favour by participating.

Of course the adult’s version of this symbol is the consent form. While issues of data privacy, security, and misuse are today widespread, we found most adults to be relaxed about their children’s participation. But some were anxious about the dangers of social media, some didn’t want their children filmed, others didn’t want to give family names or dates of birth. Collection of information from people can certainly be used for exploitative ends. Indeed the use of information derived from people without their consent can be considered a form of exploitation. In practicing ‘participatory’ action research, however, we try to generate new understandings in ways that we can all benefit.

We will therefore use our results to help the design of future exhibits at the Centre for Life. By doing data collection and research ‘in’ rather than ‘on’ the Centre for Life we hope not only that we can take part in the collective expansion of knowledge, but feed that knowledge back into the context from which we drew it to better understand and thereby improve that context for its visitors. This is our practical science.

“Copying is stupid!” (Or is it?)

by Zarja Muršič & Guy Lavender Forsyth

Imitation is one type of social learning, and it is one that humans are expert at. We are really very good at copying things around us, to the extent that some experiments have shown our willingness to copy even irrelevant, superfluous actions.

Take the example of trying to open a locked box. Imagine you are observing somebody trying to open the box. She grabs the red block on the box, then she opens the box by pulling a green string. What would you do to open the box? Grab the red block first or just the green string? In numerous cases experiments have shown our tendency to copy what we have observed and touch the red block first before opening the box by pulling the green string. This seems to remain true even when people can see that grabbing the red block does not have any functional role in opening the box. This example may be a little abstract, but in everyday life we copy others in the way we dress, eat, and act, as well as in the ways we solve problems.

In our ongoing research, using an exhibit where children build with wooden blocks, copying is clearly present as children depend on things they remember from daily life which they use to structure their buildings. We see very few buildings based upon novel abstract designs. They are overwhelmingly models of known things. These things vary wildly: from ‘army castles’ to elephants, and huts to smiley faces (complete with glasses). This copying is not restricted to depicting just the phenomena of the physical world: one boy’s ‘TNT cannon’ took inspiration from the virtual gaming world of Minecraft. And when not depicting ‘real’ things in miniature, the children tend to take their cues from (what we guess are) previous experiences with blocks similar to the ones we give them. This is especially the case with the game Jenga, which we have found to be absolutely endemic to the minds of the visiting public in the Centre for Life. Jenga appears strongly associated with the specific geometrical pattern in children. This remains true whether it be a classic Jenga tower like the ones we invariably toppled on wintry evenings as kids or, more adventurously, challenging the 90º angle tradition through addition of diagonals.

IMG_2576
Children building with wooden blocks

The deviation from the Jenga model supplied to the children is perhaps evidence of one crucial capacity of human beings. When pre-existing structures are reproduced with embellishments, culture can become cumulative. It is such cumulative accumulation of elaborations and refinements, over generations, that is what gives us cars, computers, and space travel. It is also what gives us science like that we are conducting in the Centre for Life.

Cumulative culture is widely thought to be dependent on exact copying, and so by the time we are toddlers we are already accomplished in copying both peers and, more commonly, adults. Humans have many biases in their copying, for example one study found that children copy adults more often than they do a more knowledgeable child. On the other hand, when it comes to play, children copy their peers more often than adults. Thus we can say that the setting and context in which we put the child influences the way she behaves.

We are currently studying these topics in the Centre for Life. We don’t give children puzzle boxes that are often used in psychological research (i.e. the locked box described at the beginning of this post). Instead we present children with an open-ended task involving building blocks, which offers them the opportunity to be creative. Children are faced with wooden blocks and all we ask of them is to build something.

Currently we (Guy, an undergraduate student at Durham University’s Anthropology department, and Zarja, a PhD student from the same department), are conducting a study in which we explore the ways children build together in groups, and what it is they build. We see lots of children copying each other, but at the same time innovation or invention, both of which are required for cumulative culture.

We also have lots of fun after the children build their structures, since we get to ask them some questions. One of these questions being whether they copied (or not) when building, and why they did so.

Here is a snippet of their answers regarding why they didn’t copy:

“Copying is stupid. It might be wrong, e.g. in a test. You haven’t actually learned anything.”

“She [my sister] wouldn’t like it.”

“It’s cheating.”

“It’s stealing their ideas.”

“I like my own ideas.”

We are not yet sure what these responses mean for our study. But we will keep you updated with our results. We hope what we find will enable us to see the extent to which copying and innovation do actually occur when children are working in groups, in the relaxed atmosphere of the Centre for Life.

Evolution of Methodologies

The team have been busy again this month, developing pathways from the project for impact. Claire and Hannah were invited to share learnings and speak at the Evolution of Methodologies conference on 29th June, held at The National Glass Centre in Sunderland.

80 participants gathered to consider how artists and designers are evolving practice based research at the conference funded by the Arts and Humanities Research Council  and organised by the Northumbria- Sunderland Centre for Doctoral Training in Art and Design.

Together, we critically interrogated practice-led research methodologies across creative disciplines. We discussed the Participatory Action Research methodology and design thinking practices which have informed the work to date. The slides of our presentation can be viewed here:

And the whole session can be watched on demand here (we start at 1:06:35):

https://sml.sunderland.ac.uk/player?autostart=n&fullscreen=y&width=835&height=505&videoId=2046&quality=hi&captions=n&chapterId=0

20160629_121745
Q&A session with the participants

The presentation generated a lot of discussion in the Q&A session, and the team has since been invited to contribute a chapter to a new book to be published by Routledge: ‘Transcending Disciplinarity; Reconnecting Philosophies of Research Design and Methodology in Art and Science’.

Ecsite 2016: impact through sharing

Team members Andy Lloyd and Bethan Ross shared the learnings and aspirations of the live research experiment ongoing through the interactive research pod at Centre for Life with the participants of the European conference for science centres and museums this month. The 27th Ecsite Annual Conference took place in June in Graz, Austria. It gathered 1,081 participants from 53 different countries, making it the second largest Ecsite Annual Conference ever.

As well as Beth and Andy presenting at the conference, Andy was interviewed for the European network of science centres and museums Magazine – Spokes#20 – June 2016. In it, Andy reports:

“I received a lot of interest in the live research exhibit in the Brain Zone. The experiment was devised by Anthropology researchers from Durham University. They are interested in social learning and “cumulative culture”, the way ideas are shared and passed on.  Previous research with non-human primates suggest that copying is very common, but humans are especially good at innovating and being creative. Our exhibit explores how the environmental conditions influence the creativity people demonstrate. We can change whether people are able to see and interact with each other as they play with building blocks, and record what they make. Interest was very high from other science centre people, so we have created a free pdf to show what we did and how we did it, that anyone can access here. The researchers want to track how the research ideas spread, so if you do make use of any of this we would love to hear how it goes”.

We’ve added a new Resources page to this website so that anyone can download our free guide in pdf format: How To… create an Interactive Research Pod.

Chair of Wellcome Trust visits exhibit pod

IMG_2558
Baroness Manningham-Buller chats with the research team by the exhibit.

 

The Chair of the Wellcome Trust, Baroness Manningham-Buller visited the interactive research pod, and spoke to the Design for Creativity and Innovation in Informal Science Learning team of academic researchers and science centre practitioners. The team explained to the Baroness about the live science experiments we are undertaking with it, and the research outcomes we might see. The Baroness officially opened the Brain Zone gallery at the Centre for Life, where the interactive research pod is hosted amongst other exhibits that engage people with exploring how our brains work.

The Brain Zone has had input from practitioners and designers, and also from from a wide-range of scientists and researchers, from neuroscientists to psychologists. However, our multidisciplinary team of academics from Durham University broadened out the disciplines represented to include anthropology, digital humanities, and computer scientists! Continue reading “Chair of Wellcome Trust visits exhibit pod”